WEBVTT 00:01.690 --> 00:04.310 Prof: Let's talk about biology and eating, 00:04.310 --> 00:07.040 an incredibly interesting yet complicated topic. 00:07.040 --> 00:10.170 By the time we're done with this class I hope that you, 00:10.170 --> 00:13.510 as I am, will be impressed with just how many factors are 00:13.506 --> 00:15.826 affecting what people choose to eat, 00:15.830 --> 00:18.080 how much they eat, the way they regulate their 00:18.080 --> 00:20.440 body weight, intake of certain nutrients, 00:20.436 --> 00:21.236 and the like. 00:21.240 --> 00:23.300 There are all these interesting, some cases 00:23.301 --> 00:26.001 redundant systems that are affecting what people eat. 00:26.000 --> 00:29.660 Then we're going to take the completely opposite attack next 00:29.655 --> 00:32.315 week where--in the next class on Monday, 00:32.320 --> 00:34.690 we're going to begin by talking about addiction, 00:34.690 --> 00:36.670 which is really a carry over from this, 00:36.670 --> 00:38.550 food and addiction, but there's just not enough 00:38.554 --> 00:40.074 time to cram it into this lecture, 00:40.070 --> 00:43.140 and then we're going to move onto culture and how culture 00:43.138 --> 00:44.068 affects eating. 00:44.070 --> 00:47.240 You could make a strong argument that eating is driven 00:47.237 --> 00:50.227 almost entirely by biology, or you could also make the 00:50.232 --> 00:52.732 argument that it's driven almost entirely by culture; 00:52.730 --> 00:56.070 but where it gets extremely interesting is where the two 00:56.073 --> 00:58.933 intersect and how the two affect one another. 00:58.930 --> 01:01.160 We'll talk about more of the biology today. 01:01.158 --> 01:05.218 Some years ago I was talking to a person who worked for M&M 01:05.224 --> 01:07.484 Mars, a big candy company of course, 01:07.475 --> 01:10.345 and two of their biggest selling products are Three 01:10.352 --> 01:12.542 Musketeers and Snickers candy bars. 01:12.540 --> 01:15.790 This individual told me that they're--that one of them is 01:15.793 --> 01:19.113 preferred much more by adults and the other preferred much 01:19.105 --> 01:22.475 more by children and there are properties that I'll ask you 01:22.476 --> 01:25.316 about in a minute that distinguish why one is more 01:25.322 --> 01:28.522 attractive to children and the other by adults. 01:28.519 --> 01:31.059 Let's try out the clickers today. 01:31.060 --> 01:34.220 Which do you think more attractive to the kids: 01:34.220 --> 01:37.930 the Three Musketeers--oh you know what I need to do? 01:37.930 --> 01:39.010 I'm sorry. 01:39.010 --> 01:42.670 I need to boot this up through another program. 01:42.670 --> 01:47.910 01:47.910 --> 01:48.780 I apologize. 01:48.780 --> 01:56.340 01:56.340 --> 01:59.060 See I was so worried that we had so much to do today that I 01:59.058 --> 02:00.228 forgot to boot this up. 02:00.230 --> 02:33.240 02:33.240 --> 02:34.860 Okay, this time it should work. 02:34.860 --> 02:36.820 Now you've had a chance to think about it as well. 02:36.818 --> 02:39.078 All right, so let's see if the technology is correct. 02:39.080 --> 02:42.160 What do kids prefer: the Three Musketeers or the 02:42.158 --> 02:42.878 Snickers? 02:42.878 --> 02:45.508 Go ahead and log in, make sure the green light goes 02:45.507 --> 02:45.767 on. 02:45.770 --> 02:54.240 02:54.240 --> 03:00.120 Okay so people are still voting it appears. 03:00.120 --> 03:04.920 All right, so let's stop the voting now and see if this--yeah 03:04.919 --> 03:09.239 so 49% of you said Three Musketeers, 51% said Snickers, 03:09.240 --> 03:11.800 so almost a dead heat there. 03:11.800 --> 03:14.700 Well the answer is the kids prefer the Three Musketeers, 03:14.699 --> 03:17.599 adults prefer the Snickers, who can guess why that might 03:17.599 --> 03:17.969 be? 03:17.970 --> 03:18.840 Yes. 03:18.840 --> 03:19.360 Student: > 03:19.360 --> 03:24.250 03:24.250 --> 03:25.770 Prof: All right, you got the right answer that 03:25.771 --> 03:27.061 it's the peanuts that make the difference. 03:27.060 --> 03:29.750 What is it about the peanuts do you think make adults like it 03:29.747 --> 03:30.417 and kids not? 03:30.419 --> 03:33.659 I mean kids like peanut butter after all. 03:33.660 --> 03:37.610 Texture is the answer. 03:37.610 --> 03:41.550 The peanuts introduce a texture like a Nestle's Crunch Bar would 03:41.550 --> 03:41.990 have. 03:41.990 --> 03:45.220 Kids tend to like uniformity in texture and tend not to like 03:45.215 --> 03:48.545 extra things thrown in that confuse them with the texture, 03:48.550 --> 03:51.380 whereas, adults do like things that have texture, 03:51.378 --> 03:54.888 so as much as the adults may like the taste of the peanuts in 03:54.894 --> 03:57.874 that kind of a thing, it's really the texture that 03:57.865 --> 04:00.265 makes them like that particular candy bar. 04:00.270 --> 04:02.610 So that's one little interesting little sensory 04:02.611 --> 04:05.261 property of foods that will affect who likes what. 04:05.258 --> 04:08.998 Today we're going to talk about how biology affects a number of 04:09.001 --> 04:11.841 things and these are very interesting questions: 04:11.837 --> 04:13.887 how does biology affect taste? 04:13.889 --> 04:17.139 Which foods do we prefer or not? 04:17.139 --> 04:18.979 We've talked about that some extent before. 04:18.980 --> 04:20.330 How does it affect mood? 04:20.329 --> 04:22.739 People seek out foods when they're feeling depressed, 04:22.735 --> 04:25.505 when they're feeling anxious, when they're feeling lonely. 04:25.509 --> 04:29.489 It's different for some people; how is biology affecting that? 04:29.490 --> 04:31.550 Then of course, these other things are very 04:31.548 --> 04:32.528 important as well. 04:32.528 --> 04:35.628 Just to make a brief distinction on the third bullet 04:35.629 --> 04:39.219 point about hunger and satiety, you probably know what those 04:39.218 --> 04:40.128 terms mean. 04:40.129 --> 04:43.419 Hunger is pretty obvious, satiety refers to how full a 04:43.420 --> 04:46.900 person feels and when eating shuts off once it starts. 04:46.899 --> 04:49.899 We'll come back and talk about some animal studies that 04:49.899 --> 04:53.009 distinguish hunger and satiety, human studies as well. 04:53.009 --> 04:56.579 Today we're going to talk about taste and I'm going to introduce 04:56.584 --> 04:59.884 this concept that was developed by a former Yale researcher 04:59.875 --> 05:02.255 named Linda Bartoshuk on supertasting. 05:02.259 --> 05:05.019 We're going to talk about the physiology of eating, 05:05.019 --> 05:07.559 or what the animal researchers call feeding. 05:07.560 --> 05:10.180 I'm not sure why it's feeding in animals, and eating in 05:10.175 --> 05:11.865 humans, but that's the way it is. 05:11.870 --> 05:15.170 We'll talk about energy balance in the body and how body weight 05:15.165 --> 05:17.585 gets affected, the genetics of these things, 05:17.591 --> 05:20.271 and then we'll end with this discussion of something 05:20.266 --> 05:22.256 interesting called taste aversions. 05:22.259 --> 05:24.369 First of all, taste. 05:24.370 --> 05:28.230 There's sort of this basic Biology 101 that--where people 05:28.232 --> 05:32.162 get a sense that there are different sensory properties of 05:32.163 --> 05:36.163 taste and different parts of the tongue that perceive these 05:36.163 --> 05:37.063 tastes. 05:37.060 --> 05:40.570 This has been known for years and it's easy to map out the 05:40.569 --> 05:42.539 biology of this kind of thing. 05:42.540 --> 05:44.510 This is a pretty simplistic version of it, 05:44.511 --> 05:46.921 but there's some very interesting work that's taken 05:46.915 --> 05:48.065 this to a new level. 05:48.069 --> 05:51.629 You can see how taste effects has such a profound effect on 05:51.634 --> 05:55.144 people, and what it does to them, and you see it in facial 05:55.137 --> 05:56.057 expression. 05:56.060 --> 05:57.990 You see somebody eating something sweet, 05:57.992 --> 06:00.672 fatty, that they enjoy and you get a look like that. 06:00.670 --> 06:02.990 Somebody eats something they don't like and you get a look 06:02.985 --> 06:03.265 that. 06:03.269 --> 06:06.939 And so if you just saw this face on the right and you asked 06:06.939 --> 06:09.409 people to say, well what do you think is 06:09.408 --> 06:11.178 happening with this man? 06:11.180 --> 06:14.430 People could say well he saw something disgusting or he 06:14.432 --> 06:17.812 tasted something disgusting, and that word disgust is pretty 06:17.809 --> 06:20.709 interesting in this context, but it shows up in facial 06:20.709 --> 06:21.459 expressions. 06:21.459 --> 06:23.759 Now here's the concept of supertasting. 06:23.759 --> 06:27.359 This researcher Linda Bartoshuk, a terrific scientist, 06:27.360 --> 06:29.650 who was here at Yale for many years and then moved to The 06:29.654 --> 06:32.644 University of Florida, has looked at the way the 06:32.644 --> 06:36.344 tongue and the body perceives tastes of foods. 06:36.339 --> 06:39.779 Her desire, one of her scientific desires, 06:39.779 --> 06:43.459 is to map out the biology of these things and to understand 06:43.459 --> 06:47.139 how taste affects biology and how biology affects taste. 06:47.139 --> 06:49.709 She's really, as I said, a very fine 06:49.706 --> 06:50.656 researcher. 06:50.660 --> 06:53.420 Well one of the things that she's done is work with 06:53.420 --> 06:57.220 something called PROP, and PROP is the abbreviation 06:57.221 --> 07:01.031 for this compound that you see on the bottom, 07:01.028 --> 07:02.758 Propylthiouracil. 07:02.759 --> 07:07.609 It got the--which is a variation of something that got 07:07.605 --> 07:11.625 discovered in 1931 by a chemist named Fox. 07:11.629 --> 07:15.469 He was working with that particular compound that you see 07:15.471 --> 07:19.591 in the top bullet point there, and it exploded in his lab. 07:19.589 --> 07:24.659 What happened is he found that the associate got some of this 07:24.658 --> 07:27.578 in his mouth, tasted it as quite bitter, 07:27.577 --> 07:30.987 but he didn't taste it at all, and he got interested in why 07:30.994 --> 07:32.324 would this be this case? 07:32.319 --> 07:35.419 Why would the two of them have the same thing in their mouth, 07:35.423 --> 07:37.083 but taste it much differently? 07:37.079 --> 07:39.619 It wasn't even so much that one liked it and the other one 07:39.619 --> 07:41.349 didn't; one of them couldn't even 07:41.346 --> 07:41.926 detect it. 07:41.930 --> 07:47.630 That got picked up by scientists, including Bartoshuk, 07:47.629 --> 07:52.009 who started looking at this and have this used this compound, 07:52.009 --> 07:55.319 the PROP, in many, many interesting studies. 07:55.319 --> 07:57.969 What they do is they put a little bit of this on a little 07:57.966 --> 08:00.896 piece of paper and you put the paper on your tongue and keep it 08:00.898 --> 08:02.788 in your mouth for just a short time. 08:02.790 --> 08:06.800 The population breaks down into three groups. 08:06.800 --> 08:09.460 There are groups who can't even detect it at all, 08:09.459 --> 08:11.289 they're called the non-tasters. 08:11.290 --> 08:13.710 There are some people who experience it as mildly bitter; 08:13.709 --> 08:15.899 those are the medium tasters. 08:15.899 --> 08:19.289 And then the supertasters are people who experience it as 08:19.288 --> 08:20.438 extremely bitter. 08:20.439 --> 08:22.959 The population breaks down about like this: 08:22.963 --> 08:26.453 quarter of the people are at the extremes and 50% of people 08:26.447 --> 08:27.527 in the middle. 08:27.528 --> 08:30.908 The interesting comparison is between the non-tasters and the 08:30.906 --> 08:31.746 supertasters. 08:31.750 --> 08:38.310 What Bartoshuk has done is mapped out the tongue physiology 08:38.313 --> 08:41.033 on this, and there's a part of the 08:41.033 --> 08:43.573 tongue shown in this little picture here, 08:43.570 --> 08:48.580 of where there's a high density of these things called fungiform 08:48.575 --> 08:49.365 papilla. 08:49.370 --> 08:54.230 Bartoshuk and others have shown that the ability to detect this 08:54.230 --> 08:58.700 particular chemical resides in that part of the tongue. 08:58.700 --> 09:02.030 What you do is just put a little dye on the tongue and 09:02.033 --> 09:05.833 then you take a picture of it, and you can actually count, 09:05.833 --> 09:09.353 once it's magnified, the number of these fungiform 09:09.346 --> 09:09.996 papilla. 09:10.000 --> 09:13.880 If you look at the picture, but the comparison of tasters 09:13.875 --> 09:17.125 and non-tasters, you get something like this. 09:17.129 --> 09:20.759 So at that part of the tongue each of those white dots would 09:20.758 --> 09:23.278 represent one of the fungiform papilla. 09:23.278 --> 09:27.418 The supertasters--again the ones who detect that chemical as 09:27.423 --> 09:31.643 very bitter--look like that, so really quite a difference. 09:31.639 --> 09:36.279 You get this interesting sense between 25% of the population 09:36.275 --> 09:39.545 looks like that, 25% looks like the top slide 09:39.549 --> 09:43.429 and you could see how people--to the extent this is related to 09:43.429 --> 09:47.119 food preferences--how the tongue people are born with, 09:47.120 --> 09:50.230 and there's probably related physiology elsewhere in the body 09:50.229 --> 09:52.329 to that, how that's going to affect what 09:52.327 --> 09:52.917 people eat. 09:52.918 --> 09:57.038 Now what she--what Linda Bartoshuk has found is that the 09:57.042 --> 10:01.092 people who were the supertasters in that 25% group, 10:01.090 --> 10:03.210 and to a lesser extent but still true, 10:03.210 --> 10:05.630 of that 50% in the middle, the larger group, 10:05.629 --> 10:09.869 they perceive an intense burn from things that are considered 10:09.870 --> 10:13.620 food irritants and they also perceive the most intense 10:13.618 --> 10:15.808 sweetness from sweet foods. 10:15.808 --> 10:19.478 Bitter vegetables, like broccoli and cauliflower 10:19.482 --> 10:24.252 for example, are experienced as very bitter by that 25% of the 10:24.250 --> 10:26.830 population, the supertasters. 10:26.830 --> 10:30.440 The non-tasters don't experience them as bitter or 10:30.440 --> 10:31.840 difficult at all. 10:31.840 --> 10:35.540 You can imagine how easy it is for the non-tasters to eat those 10:35.542 --> 10:38.292 kinds of things and how difficult it is for the 10:38.288 --> 10:42.088 supertasters to eat those, and it also has relationships 10:42.085 --> 10:43.355 to desire for fat. 10:43.360 --> 10:45.680 The sweetness is interesting too. 10:45.678 --> 10:48.328 If Linda were here doing a demonstration, 10:48.330 --> 10:51.230 she would have you all put this on your tongue, 10:51.230 --> 10:53.150 see which group you happen to fall in, 10:53.149 --> 10:55.209 give you a little butterscotch candy, 10:55.210 --> 10:58.200 and then have you rate how sweet it is and how pleasant 10:58.202 --> 10:59.092 that taste is. 10:59.090 --> 11:03.010 She could predict in advance who would experience it as very 11:03.014 --> 11:06.224 sweet and very pleasant, depending on whether you were a 11:06.216 --> 11:08.616 supertaster or not, with the supertasters 11:08.620 --> 11:12.840 experiencing the sweetness as very desirable and very powerful 11:12.839 --> 11:14.359 sensory experience. 11:14.360 --> 11:20.600 Coupled together is this turn-off to the irritants, 11:20.600 --> 11:22.860 the fresh fruits and vegetables, not fruits so much 11:22.861 --> 11:25.131 because they're sweet but to the bitter vegetables, 11:25.125 --> 11:26.885 and then a liking for sweet things. 11:26.889 --> 11:29.659 Of course this would affect dietary preferences and 11:29.664 --> 11:30.724 therefore health. 11:30.720 --> 11:34.170 This is a very interesting phenomenon that she has found. 11:34.168 --> 11:37.228 The way she puts it in more generic terms is this: 11:37.225 --> 11:40.775 that the supertasters live on--in this neon world of taste 11:40.779 --> 11:44.209 where everything is bright and vivid, vivid and bold and 11:44.208 --> 11:47.528 potent; whereas the non-tasters live in 11:47.528 --> 11:51.978 this pastel world where they're not experiencing as much 11:51.977 --> 11:56.107 preference for sweets nor the aversion to the bitter 11:56.105 --> 11:58.285 vegetables and things. 11:58.288 --> 12:01.178 And so you would expect from that point of view for the 12:01.182 --> 12:03.972 supertasters to have greater problem with weight, 12:03.970 --> 12:07.000 greater problems with diet related to health than is the 12:06.998 --> 12:08.538 case with the non-tasters. 12:08.538 --> 12:12.568 That begins to give you a sense of what's involved just with the 12:12.566 --> 12:14.416 taste part of the equation. 12:14.418 --> 12:17.278 Now let's talk about the physiology of eating. 12:17.278 --> 12:20.498 Now, I'm going to break this down for you. 12:20.500 --> 12:22.860 It took me a lot of time to uncouple all the little pieces 12:22.860 --> 12:24.720 in this graph, but I'm going to show it to you 12:24.722 --> 12:25.512 piece by piece. 12:25.509 --> 12:30.329 This is one conceptualization of all the factors that are 12:30.328 --> 12:34.458 affecting taste and affecting what people eat. 12:34.460 --> 12:37.600 Central to this are central signals those--they would be the 12:37.596 --> 12:40.676 brain and the central nervous system and then over here are 12:40.682 --> 12:43.242 peripheral signals that happen outside the brain, 12:43.235 --> 12:45.145 mainly from the body's organs. 12:45.149 --> 12:49.369 Let's break this down and take it piece by piece and show you 12:49.369 --> 12:51.689 how it might all fall together. 12:51.690 --> 12:53.380 Let's start with the brain. 12:53.379 --> 12:56.199 First of all, the brain and its subsequent 12:56.202 --> 13:00.132 physiological reaction is affected by external factors. 13:00.129 --> 13:03.089 The fact that you might seek out certain foods to soothe 13:03.091 --> 13:06.221 yourself when you're stressed means that there are external 13:06.215 --> 13:09.225 things going on that make certain foods more desirable to 13:09.230 --> 13:12.570 you at some times then others, that would be an example. 13:12.570 --> 13:16.640 There are other things like what's going on in the 13:16.643 --> 13:19.253 environment, as we used as an example 13:19.245 --> 13:22.125 before, you just had a nice meal at a restaurant, 13:22.129 --> 13:24.249 you don't feel like you could eat one more calorie, 13:24.250 --> 13:26.610 here comes the dessert cart and all of sudden you're hungry 13:26.613 --> 13:28.443 again; those are external factors that 13:28.442 --> 13:30.722 affect the brain and therefore the physiology. 13:30.720 --> 13:33.970 There are central signals and peripheral signals, 13:33.972 --> 13:35.602 as I mentioned before. 13:35.600 --> 13:37.510 Let's look at the central signals first. 13:37.509 --> 13:39.799 There are signals that are going on in the brain, 13:39.798 --> 13:43.008 some of which stimulate eating and others inhibit eating, 13:43.009 --> 13:45.629 and that's the hunger and satiety that we mentioned a 13:45.625 --> 13:46.275 moment ago. 13:46.279 --> 13:49.669 These things affect--directly affect food intake. 13:49.668 --> 13:53.258 How much food intake--what people eat and what kind of 13:53.259 --> 13:55.659 foods they eat, have a feedback loop to the 13:55.660 --> 13:58.150 brain that affects subsequent intake, so you get this loop 13:58.154 --> 13:58.684 going on. 13:58.678 --> 14:02.368 From a common sense point of view, if somebody eats a lot of 14:02.371 --> 14:05.501 food, then the feedback loop would go to the brain, 14:05.500 --> 14:07.880 I've had enough it's time to stop. 14:07.879 --> 14:11.589 There are signals--or I haven't had enough and I need to eat 14:11.587 --> 14:15.487 more and these central signals that stimulate or inhibit eating 14:15.485 --> 14:17.555 are--fall into that category. 14:17.558 --> 14:21.038 There are certain number of chemicals in the body that 14:21.038 --> 14:23.858 stimulate eating, certain ones that decrease 14:23.861 --> 14:27.211 eating, and I'll just point out a few of these. 14:27.210 --> 14:29.680 You don't really need to know what these are but I'll point 14:29.682 --> 14:30.922 out just a few as examples. 14:30.918 --> 14:34.598 One would be the NPY which stands for Neuropeptide Y, 14:34.597 --> 14:38.767 and the 5HT which is something that you will hear more of in 14:38.772 --> 14:41.392 the common language as serotonin. 14:41.389 --> 14:47.029 It's very interesting how these two things affect--affect 14:47.032 --> 14:50.062 eating, and the SSRI is the selective 14:50.058 --> 14:54.308 serotonin reuptake inhibitors are a class of drugs mainly used 14:54.306 --> 14:55.556 for depression. 14:55.558 --> 15:00.458 Prozac and drugs like that are SSRI drugs. 15:00.460 --> 15:03.240 Those drugs, once they were discovered, 15:03.240 --> 15:06.690 the laboratory scientists found in the initial animal studies 15:06.692 --> 15:10.092 just looking at whether these drugs were safe or not for the 15:10.086 --> 15:12.996 health of the animals, found out that the animals lost 15:12.996 --> 15:15.226 weight and there was some suppression of appetite. 15:15.230 --> 15:18.100 Then scientists said, well boy if you think there's a 15:18.096 --> 15:21.016 market for depression, just think about the market for 15:21.018 --> 15:21.898 weight loss. 15:21.899 --> 15:24.889 There was some marketing of these drugs for that at one 15:24.886 --> 15:27.816 point but they haven't really worked out that well. 15:27.820 --> 15:31.990 You can see how these things are affecting food intake in 15:31.993 --> 15:34.233 feedback loops to the brain. 15:34.230 --> 15:37.660 People eat, how much they eat is determined partly by some of 15:37.663 --> 15:40.203 these things, what they eat then affects 15:40.202 --> 15:42.482 those things, and you get the feedback loop 15:42.477 --> 15:42.937 going on. 15:42.940 --> 15:47.550 There's this--what happens between what people eat and then 15:47.548 --> 15:50.198 subsequently, what their brain tells them to 15:50.198 --> 15:53.048 eat is a very interesting part of the equation and that's where 15:53.048 --> 15:55.208 the--some of the peripheral signals come in. 15:55.210 --> 15:57.790 Your body takes in food, it senses that you've had 15:57.785 --> 15:59.675 nutrients of one sort or another, 15:59.678 --> 16:02.668 a series of metabolic processes kick in, 16:02.668 --> 16:05.798 and those affect what happens to the brain subsequently. 16:05.798 --> 16:07.768 Here's some of the things that fall in those, 16:07.765 --> 16:09.325 those are the peripheral signals. 16:09.330 --> 16:14.710 These are some of the compounds that determine--that are the 16:14.712 --> 16:16.632 peripheral signals. 16:16.629 --> 16:20.079 We talked about blood glucose last week when we--or on Monday 16:20.077 --> 16:22.487 when we talked about the glycemic index. 16:22.490 --> 16:24.740 You eat food, your blood sugar goes up, 16:24.739 --> 16:28.469 that's the blood glucose goes up, and that affects the brain. 16:28.470 --> 16:31.760 We talked about hunger and there was the spike with the 16:31.764 --> 16:34.814 high glycemic index foods, the lower spike with low 16:34.813 --> 16:38.173 glycemic index foods; so the glucose that you see 16:38.168 --> 16:40.468 over here is one example of that. 16:40.470 --> 16:44.090 Insulin is also very involved, as you might imagine, 16:44.090 --> 16:47.350 and then hormones that get talked about more in the press, 16:47.350 --> 16:50.740 Ghrelin and Leptin are important players here, 16:50.740 --> 16:52.100 and then Cortisol which is a stress hormone, 16:52.096 --> 16:52.786 is a player as well. 16:52.789 --> 16:58.089 These act through organs. 16:58.090 --> 17:01.410 It's the effect of food on different organ systems in the 17:01.413 --> 17:05.333 body that create the release or the suppression of these things. 17:05.328 --> 17:08.848 First the liver is involved in the glucose, so food intake will 17:08.851 --> 17:12.261 affect the liver that will determine blood glucose levels. 17:12.259 --> 17:16.299 You have the gastrointestinal tract involved as well, 17:16.298 --> 17:20.188 so of course your small and large intestine in the stomach 17:20.193 --> 17:22.463 are involved here, and those will affect these 17:22.463 --> 17:23.703 things, especially insulin and Ghrelin. 17:23.700 --> 17:28.250 Then you have the adipose tissue. 17:28.250 --> 17:31.170 The fat cells are involved because they're perceiving 17:31.173 --> 17:34.833 signals from the incoming food; those affect Leptin. 17:34.828 --> 17:38.578 Then you have adrenal glands that affect the Cortisol. 17:38.578 --> 17:42.948 Cortisol, a stress hormone, is an interesting player here. 17:42.950 --> 17:45.150 One of your teaching fellows, Alli Crum, 17:45.150 --> 17:47.850 will be talking about some of the work that she's doing in a 17:47.853 --> 17:50.693 later class and she's doing some work that involves a number of 17:50.694 --> 17:53.054 these things, a very interesting study that 17:53.047 --> 17:56.047 looks at the intersection of psychology and perceptions in 17:56.049 --> 17:56.629 biology. 17:56.630 --> 18:01.010 These things then have signals that would either increase or 18:01.005 --> 18:04.485 decrease eating, and they feed back to the brain 18:04.490 --> 18:06.270 in that kind of way. 18:06.269 --> 18:08.709 So you put all this system together and you get things like 18:08.713 --> 18:09.013 this. 18:09.009 --> 18:12.629 This is what happens after food is consumed and this--then it 18:12.630 --> 18:16.190 goes back to the brain and the brain--then that affects what 18:16.190 --> 18:17.880 people eat subsequently. 18:17.880 --> 18:21.060 If you put it all together you have this, what seems like 18:21.060 --> 18:23.050 perplexing picture to begin with. 18:23.048 --> 18:26.128 It still might be perplexing but the--what I'm hoping that 18:26.132 --> 18:29.382 you take away from this is an appreciation for how many parts 18:29.375 --> 18:32.075 of the body are affected when people eat food; 18:32.078 --> 18:35.428 how the brain is an important player and what people choose to 18:35.426 --> 18:38.126 eat in the beginning; and how all these feedback 18:38.133 --> 18:39.303 loops are occurring. 18:39.298 --> 18:43.148 It's even more complicated then this, but this is an interesting 18:43.151 --> 18:45.231 start at looking at the picture. 18:45.230 --> 18:49.860 Let's look at some factors that affect short-term food intake. 18:49.858 --> 18:52.718 Now long term food intake, how much people are eating 18:52.723 --> 18:55.713 overall, is affected by some overlapping 18:55.705 --> 18:59.065 but also different factors, but let's talk about what 18:59.071 --> 19:02.321 people eat in the short term, like in a given meal at a given 19:02.316 --> 19:03.196 point in time. 19:03.200 --> 19:07.030 There are different ways scientists have looked at this 19:07.025 --> 19:11.275 and they've measured different patterns of what people eat by 19:11.276 --> 19:14.036 breaking things down into this way. 19:14.038 --> 19:17.688 What factors determine whether a meal gets started? 19:17.690 --> 19:22.280 What determines whether you start eating at a given point? 19:22.279 --> 19:24.029 When do you become hungry? 19:24.028 --> 19:28.308 What triggers some awareness in your mind that you're ready to 19:28.308 --> 19:28.728 eat? 19:28.730 --> 19:32.350 That would determine meal initiation or snack initiation, 19:32.346 --> 19:33.246 or whatever. 19:33.250 --> 19:36.370 The size of the meal of course is an important factor, 19:36.368 --> 19:40.398 and there are real differences here between the meal initiation 19:40.395 --> 19:43.385 and the meal size, and then the meal termination 19:43.387 --> 19:44.677 comes in here as well. 19:44.680 --> 19:47.330 There have been very interesting studies over the 19:47.332 --> 19:50.812 years using different drugs with animals to look at what it does 19:50.814 --> 19:52.034 to their appetite. 19:52.029 --> 19:55.339 Of course the pharmaceutical companies have done thousands 19:55.343 --> 19:58.773 and thousands of these studies in hope of finding drugs that 19:58.773 --> 20:02.033 will help people with their eating and their weight. 20:02.028 --> 20:05.578 When you do studies with animals you find that some drugs 20:05.579 --> 20:09.219 affect the initiation of a meal, they affect hunger, 20:09.217 --> 20:13.317 so how likely is it for the animal (or a human) to start 20:13.318 --> 20:15.458 eating, and that would be the hunger 20:15.461 --> 20:16.031 part of it. 20:16.028 --> 20:19.258 Some drugs selectively affect that but they don't affect how 20:19.256 --> 20:21.276 much is eaten once the meal starts. 20:21.278 --> 20:25.758 What might go up then is the interval between meals or even 20:25.760 --> 20:29.780 how many meals are eaten, but what's during a meal is 20:29.778 --> 20:32.018 untouched by these drugs. 20:32.019 --> 20:34.809 Then there are a whole other class of drugs that affects 20:34.811 --> 20:35.321 satiety. 20:35.318 --> 20:38.428 The interval between meals remains untouched; 20:38.430 --> 20:42.060 the likelihood of initiating a meal remains untouched; 20:42.058 --> 20:45.458 but how much the organism eats once the meal starts is highly 20:45.457 --> 20:46.757 affected by the drug. 20:46.759 --> 20:49.959 That leads us to think that when people have problems 20:49.963 --> 20:53.613 controlling their eating, there could be some people who 20:53.605 --> 20:57.495 have disorders of hunger--or not disorders necessarily because 20:57.502 --> 21:00.482 that makes it too clinical; but problems with hunger and 21:00.482 --> 21:02.272 other people might have problems with satiety. 21:02.269 --> 21:05.139 Think in your own experience, when there are times when you 21:05.136 --> 21:07.946 are eating things you don't think you should eat or you're 21:07.954 --> 21:11.074 eating more then you think you should eat, what's going on? 21:11.069 --> 21:13.539 Is it the hunger? 21:13.538 --> 21:16.198 Do you just want to eat all the time or do you want to eat more 21:16.204 --> 21:16.554 often? 21:16.548 --> 21:19.338 Or are you eating at regular times but you're just having 21:19.339 --> 21:22.329 trouble eating as much as you think you should and you overdo 21:22.328 --> 21:25.068 it and there could be combinations of these things. 21:25.068 --> 21:28.738 That's one interesting broad cut at this whole issue. 21:28.740 --> 21:31.320 There are things that affect all of these. 21:31.318 --> 21:34.298 For example, hunger, the biological 21:34.301 --> 21:37.991 experience of hunger, will affect both meal 21:37.986 --> 21:40.526 initiation and meal size. 21:40.529 --> 21:43.679 The appetite, which is what a person's desire 21:43.683 --> 21:47.053 is for particular nutrients or particular foods, 21:47.053 --> 21:50.283 will affect both of these things as well. 21:50.279 --> 21:54.939 Satiety, which is the sense of feeling full, 21:54.940 --> 21:57.450 as I mentioned, will affect when a meal 21:57.445 --> 21:59.905 terminates, when the next meal will start 21:59.905 --> 22:03.025 because that feeling of full will only last so long and then 22:03.034 --> 22:06.324 of course that affect--that will affect the meal frequency. 22:06.318 --> 22:10.128 Then there's a concept that I'm going to explain in a minute 22:10.131 --> 22:14.071 called sensory specific satiety that will affect meal size and 22:14.073 --> 22:15.433 meal termination. 22:15.430 --> 22:19.470 When you add all these things up you have this array of arrows 22:19.470 --> 22:21.060 going every which way. 22:21.058 --> 22:25.368 Again, you can get a sense of how the biology affects actual 22:25.374 --> 22:28.304 eating patterns in these--this--this very 22:28.299 --> 22:32.689 interesting network of affects and they come together in very 22:32.686 --> 22:34.366 interesting ways. 22:34.368 --> 22:38.508 Let me tell you what sensory specific satiety is in a minute, 22:38.509 --> 22:41.569 but before we do that, I'm going to have you use your 22:41.573 --> 22:44.523 clickers and to get your sense of another issue. 22:44.519 --> 22:49.349 Here's a product called Munchies, let's put this into a 22:49.353 --> 22:51.953 specific context of a food. 22:51.950 --> 22:55.320 Frito-Lay, I don't know how long ago, 22:55.318 --> 22:58.968 at least a couple years or more, introduced a line of snack 22:58.965 --> 23:02.605 products called Munchies and there was one in particular in 23:02.612 --> 23:06.072 this chartreuse bag that was oriented towards kids. 23:06.068 --> 23:08.818 Its Munchies Kids Mix, and each of these Munchies 23:08.815 --> 23:12.015 products takes a series of things that Frito-Lay may sell 23:12.017 --> 23:15.217 anyway and puts them into a combination, into a mix. 23:15.220 --> 23:18.720 Now this one in particular, this Munchies Kids Mix, 23:18.715 --> 23:22.905 if you look at the back of the bag here's what's put together 23:22.911 --> 23:24.031 in this mix. 23:24.028 --> 23:28.298 It's Rold Gold pretzels, it's Captain Crunch Cereal 23:28.300 --> 23:31.100 right under that, it's Doritos, 23:31.104 --> 23:34.054 it's popcorn, Smart Food Popcorn, 23:34.048 --> 23:37.608 it's Cheetos, the cheese ball things, 23:37.608 --> 23:40.798 and then it's M&M type candies. 23:40.798 --> 23:43.818 These are what are going into this particular snack mix. 23:43.818 --> 23:48.078 Now, is this snack mix good or not good for kids compared to 23:48.076 --> 23:50.886 what they might be eating otherwise, 23:50.890 --> 23:55.040 which might just be one of these things put together--put 23:55.044 --> 23:55.864 in a bag? 23:55.858 --> 24:01.688 I'll talk to you a little bit about how the Munchies is being 24:01.692 --> 24:04.172 marketed, and Frito-Lay has several 24:04.172 --> 24:07.372 different messages on the bag that make it look like its going 24:07.373 --> 24:09.423 to be a healthier product for kids, 24:09.420 --> 24:11.930 and that there's something, somehow good about this 24:11.932 --> 24:13.542 compared to the normal things. 24:13.538 --> 24:18.178 Let's compare what would happen here, 24:18.180 --> 24:22.410 so I'd like you to use your clickers and we'll--on the next 24:22.406 --> 24:26.846 slide--to see whether you think that if you just sat kids down 24:26.853 --> 24:31.543 in a whatever kind of a setting, and gave them unlimited amounts 24:31.539 --> 24:34.109 of that and unlimited amounts of that, 24:34.108 --> 24:36.108 what they would eat more of and why? 24:36.108 --> 24:38.248 Let's see how your intuition works here. 24:38.250 --> 24:40.720 There's something that I mentioned in an earlier class 24:40.715 --> 24:43.085 that was kind of a tip off as to the answer here. 24:43.088 --> 24:46.968 The question is how many of you believe that people would eat 24:46.970 --> 24:49.100 more Munchies, how many of you believe they 24:49.099 --> 24:51.479 would eat more Cheetos, or how many believe it would be 24:51.479 --> 24:51.949 the same? 24:51.950 --> 24:53.760 Vote one, two, or three here. 24:53.759 --> 25:01.999 25:02.000 --> 25:05.490 Okay, a few people still voting, more Munchies, 25:05.492 --> 25:07.622 more Cheetos, or the same? 25:07.619 --> 25:10.279 Okay let's look at the graph; let's see what you guys said. 25:10.278 --> 25:14.528 Okay, 78% of you said that it was more Munchies, 25:14.530 --> 25:16.340 why would that be? 25:16.338 --> 25:21.028 Variety that was the tip off, so I'm glad you guys remembered 25:21.031 --> 25:21.581 that. 25:21.578 --> 25:23.798 It was nice that you were paying attention early that 25:23.801 --> 25:24.231 morning. 25:24.230 --> 25:28.490 Variety is one issue and variety connects with this 25:28.486 --> 25:33.166 concept of sensory specific satiety that I'd now like to 25:33.167 --> 25:34.187 explain. 25:34.190 --> 25:40.340 Sensory specific satiety is what happens when an organism 25:40.336 --> 25:44.286 eats the same food over and over, 25:44.288 --> 25:48.328 and basically what it means is that the palpability or the 25:48.333 --> 25:52.593 likeability of certain foods and desire for them will go down 25:52.588 --> 25:55.638 more than for foods that are not eaten. 25:55.640 --> 25:59.760 Presumably, this is the evolutionary response to the 25:59.763 --> 26:03.083 body's need for a variety of nutrients. 26:03.078 --> 26:05.278 If you eat the same thing over and over again, 26:05.278 --> 26:06.858 even though you might really like it, 26:06.858 --> 26:08.468 you're only getting certain nutrients, 26:08.470 --> 26:10.680 you're being deprived of others, and hence, 26:10.680 --> 26:13.280 the body sends out this signal that hey I've had enough of that 26:13.279 --> 26:15.209 stuff it's time to move onto something else. 26:15.210 --> 26:18.890 That's the sensory specific satiety concept and it's been 26:18.891 --> 26:20.931 studied a lot over the years. 26:20.930 --> 26:24.780 There's a researcher at Penn State named Barbara Rolls and 26:24.775 --> 26:29.155 I'll discuss some of her work later on in a different context, 26:29.160 --> 26:32.110 but she has done some terrific studies on sensory specific 26:32.112 --> 26:32.632 satiety. 26:32.630 --> 26:37.700 The environment thwarts sensory specific satiety doesn't it? 26:37.700 --> 26:42.250 Again, if we look back to when I was a boy, and think about 26:42.252 --> 26:46.182 when you guys were kids, the number of food choices 26:46.178 --> 26:48.218 available went way up. 26:48.220 --> 26:51.870 There's not only the number of opportunities to eat has gone 26:51.865 --> 26:55.565 way up because there's food as we mentioned before in the gas 26:55.574 --> 26:59.534 stations and the drugstores and schools and everywhere else; 26:59.529 --> 27:02.209 but the variety of foods has gone up. 27:02.210 --> 27:06.550 Each year the food industry introduces several thousand new 27:06.551 --> 27:07.751 food products. 27:07.750 --> 27:10.890 Some of them fall out of the market but some of them don't, 27:10.890 --> 27:13.240 and as a consequence, the number of options available 27:13.240 --> 27:15.590 today in a supermarket or a convenience store is way, 27:15.592 --> 27:17.132 way higher then it used to be. 27:17.130 --> 27:20.570 You think of things like this: a restaurant like the 27:20.573 --> 27:23.273 Cheesecake Factory with an enormous menu, 27:23.273 --> 27:26.653 or you go to a diner they have enormous menus. 27:26.650 --> 27:29.560 Anyplace you go it's really--if you go to a restaurant and they 27:29.559 --> 27:32.329 had like six choices you'd think you were getting gypped and 27:32.328 --> 27:35.098 there was something wrong with this place because they don't 27:35.098 --> 27:36.318 have enough choices. 27:36.318 --> 27:38.468 Then of course, you've got the variety in 27:38.465 --> 27:41.395 places like this, it's happening around the world 27:41.404 --> 27:44.704 where you have many, many choices of things in 27:44.703 --> 27:45.813 supermarkets. 27:45.808 --> 27:49.268 In fact, one thing that people notice when they come from poor 27:49.272 --> 27:50.582 countries to the U.S. 27:50.578 --> 27:52.918 of how many choices we have of a given food. 27:52.920 --> 27:55.320 So for example, they often find themselves 27:55.317 --> 27:57.537 perplexed by the number of cereals, 27:57.538 --> 28:00.288 or the number of different kinds of coffee, 28:00.288 --> 28:03.638 or how many yogurts there are in a supermarket. 28:03.640 --> 28:06.680 And it's this dizzying array of choices whereas in other 28:06.682 --> 28:09.452 countries they have fewer choices and that actually 28:09.450 --> 28:12.550 would--you would consider them being helped by that. 28:12.549 --> 28:13.799 Do you have a question? 28:13.798 --> 28:14.448 Student: > 28:14.450 --> 28:24.500 28:24.500 --> 28:25.530 Prof: Okay, that's a good question. 28:25.528 --> 28:28.208 The question is with kids especially does the desire for 28:28.210 --> 28:30.790 variety override the other sensory properties that you 28:30.794 --> 28:33.284 might get from a high sugar or a high fat food? 28:33.279 --> 28:36.159 These things are all working in competition with each other in 28:36.160 --> 28:36.680 some way. 28:36.680 --> 28:41.750 It seems from the bare reality of the fact that the population 28:41.750 --> 28:44.140 is getting heavier, and heavier, 28:44.144 --> 28:47.294 and heavier especially kids, that the things like the fat 28:47.294 --> 28:50.954 and the sugar are overriding the sensory specific satiety. 28:50.950 --> 28:53.810 Now the sensory specific satiety would probably have a 28:53.811 --> 28:57.271 chance to kick in if there were a narrower range of high fat, 28:57.269 --> 29:00.119 high sugar choices, because then the kid would get 29:00.122 --> 29:02.162 tired of those particular things; 29:02.160 --> 29:05.080 but there are so many choices that the sensory specific 29:05.076 --> 29:08.096 satiety probably never really has a chance to kick in. 29:08.098 --> 29:11.508 You see things like this happening in all parts of the 29:11.510 --> 29:15.310 world, so of course the food choices are really dizzying. 29:15.308 --> 29:18.348 Now it's interesting how Frito-Lay marketed this 29:18.351 --> 29:19.711 particular product. 29:19.710 --> 29:24.740 First, on the package it says eight vitamins--fortified with 29:24.743 --> 29:28.673 eight vitamins and minerals--essential ones. 29:28.670 --> 29:31.740 Now what do you guys think about that? 29:31.740 --> 29:36.150 Is this a reasonable thing for a company to do? 29:36.150 --> 29:38.690 Okay why? 29:38.690 --> 29:42.140 Student: > 29:42.140 --> 29:44.650 Prof: Okay, so one vote would be yes this 29:44.651 --> 29:47.271 is okay because vitamins and minerals are good. 29:47.269 --> 29:49.729 If kids are going to eat it anyway, why not throw some of 29:49.730 --> 29:50.170 them in? 29:50.170 --> 29:51.940 Okay what about some other reactions to this? 29:51.940 --> 29:53.820 Yes. 29:53.818 --> 29:54.458 Student: > 29:54.460 --> 29:58.590 29:58.588 --> 30:00.698 Prof: Okay, maybe the body's not going to 30:00.700 --> 30:02.900 use these vitamins anyway so what use are they? 30:02.900 --> 30:03.660 Yes. 30:03.660 --> 30:07.250 Student: > 30:07.250 --> 30:08.420 Prof: Yes, I'll talk about that in just a 30:08.415 --> 30:08.635 minute? 30:08.640 --> 30:10.160 Student: > 30:10.160 --> 30:10.910 Prof: Yes. 30:10.910 --> 30:11.480 Student: > 30:11.480 --> 30:15.870 30:15.868 --> 30:19.198 Prof: Okay so the--this comment was that this is 30:19.203 --> 30:22.973 probably not good because there is more harm--I'm assuming you 30:22.971 --> 30:25.751 mean from eating the high calorie foods, 30:25.750 --> 30:28.170 then you get the good from coming from the vitamins and 30:28.174 --> 30:28.674 minerals. 30:28.670 --> 30:30.550 This is an interesting thing. 30:30.548 --> 30:32.308 Now food companies do this a lot. 30:32.308 --> 30:35.008 They fortify foods, and there are cases where the 30:35.009 --> 30:37.089 fortification is a very good thing. 30:37.088 --> 30:39.958 For example, we mentioned before, 30:39.961 --> 30:45.081 putting iodine in salt helps prevent certain deficiencies, 30:45.075 --> 30:50.365 folic acid and certain things helps birth deficiencies; 30:50.368 --> 30:53.168 so there's some examples of where fortification is a good 30:53.173 --> 30:56.133 public health move and there's general consensus that it's a 30:56.127 --> 30:56.827 good idea. 30:56.828 --> 30:59.598 In this case, the vitamins and minerals don't 30:59.598 --> 31:03.438 need to be in there because (a) American children for the most 31:03.438 --> 31:06.458 part are not vitamin and mineral deficient, 31:06.460 --> 31:09.940 and (b) if you want to deliver more vitamins and minerals to 31:09.942 --> 31:13.902 American children this would not be the vehicle you would choose. 31:13.900 --> 31:20.590 Some critics of these kind of practices say that this is just 31:20.587 --> 31:23.297 pure, base marketing to make what 31:23.298 --> 31:27.008 otherwise is not a very good food seem healthier then it 31:27.006 --> 31:29.376 really is, and make it--it gives 31:29.383 --> 31:31.843 permission for parents to buy this. 31:31.838 --> 31:33.778 It says yes, this is a good thing to buy 31:33.779 --> 31:35.919 because it's going to help your children. 31:35.920 --> 31:37.680 That's what the critics would say. 31:37.680 --> 31:41.600 Now over on the right hand side up there you see that there's 31:41.596 --> 31:43.226 more permission giving. 31:43.230 --> 31:47.120 It says that kids will love this--sorry this is cut off, 31:47.124 --> 31:49.884 but it said, this is something that both 31:49.884 --> 31:52.014 kids and mothers can love. 31:52.009 --> 31:55.759 It gives mothers permission to buy it and it has all their 31:55.760 --> 31:59.870 favorite snacks in one bag; and then down here you see that 31:59.868 --> 32:04.128 it's eight vitamins and minerals again, no Trans fat and it's 32:04.125 --> 32:06.675 endorsed by Dr. Kenneth Cooper. 32:06.680 --> 32:10.010 Cooper is the father of aerobics. 32:10.009 --> 32:12.969 He's a physician who has a large practice in Dallas, 32:12.970 --> 32:17.130 a place called the Cooper Center and he was really the--he 32:17.125 --> 32:20.395 was a cardiologist, still is a cardiologist, 32:20.397 --> 32:23.457 but he invented the concept of aerobics many, 32:23.458 --> 32:24.708 many years ago. 32:24.710 --> 32:26.630 He's gotten in on the nutrition field as well. 32:26.630 --> 32:30.610 Well, he started working with PepsiCo which is the parent 32:30.613 --> 32:32.253 company to Frito-Lay. 32:32.250 --> 32:35.280 In fact, we'll have a terrific person from PepsiCo as one of 32:35.284 --> 32:38.524 our guest speakers later in the class and he can talk about this 32:38.523 --> 32:39.453 sort of thing. 32:39.450 --> 32:41.840 Cooper then gets paid as a consultant, 32:41.838 --> 32:44.038 I don't know how much, but he gets paid as a 32:44.044 --> 32:46.864 consultant and then issues his proclamation that this is 32:46.863 --> 32:48.353 somehow a healthier food. 32:48.348 --> 32:51.498 But if, in fact, kids are eating more of it, 32:51.500 --> 32:53.860 and getting more calories, more fat, 32:53.858 --> 32:56.388 and more sugar then they would if they were just eating the 32:56.387 --> 32:58.747 regular old food, then this does seem like 32:58.748 --> 33:00.608 marketing that's ill-advised. 33:00.608 --> 33:04.048 Putting out a product and then marketing it as somehow healthy 33:04.053 --> 33:06.653 when kids might eat more of it is a problem. 33:06.650 --> 33:09.840 Okay, well we did a little study on this that we haven't 33:09.843 --> 33:12.363 yet published, but we did a study where we 33:12.355 --> 33:15.585 brought kids into the lab, we found out which of these 33:15.590 --> 33:19.200 foods they liked the most and then gave them that versus the 33:19.198 --> 33:22.438 Munchies and--to see which they would eat more of. 33:22.440 --> 33:25.030 You guys were exactly right: they didn't eat less of the 33:25.030 --> 33:27.670 Munchies, they didn't eat the same, but they ate more. 33:27.670 --> 33:33.150 This creates interesting issues with marketing and biology and 33:33.147 --> 33:36.917 the whole sensory specific satiety idea. 33:36.920 --> 33:40.940 Now I'd like turn our attention to the effect of things like 33:40.942 --> 33:44.422 stress and a related concept on social position, 33:44.420 --> 33:45.730 because a study came out recently on this, 33:45.734 --> 33:46.574 it was very interesting. 33:46.568 --> 33:50.858 There's been a lot of interest in the influence of stress on 33:50.855 --> 33:54.845 eating and stress affects eating, as I mentioned before, 33:54.851 --> 33:56.451 in different ways. 33:56.450 --> 33:58.350 Let's just get a show of hands on this, 33:58.348 --> 34:01.628 if you're stressed--now most people when they're severely 34:01.625 --> 34:05.305 stressed like things are really just out of control in life tend 34:05.310 --> 34:07.710 to eat less and eating is suppressed. 34:07.710 --> 34:09.950 But at more moderate levels of stress, 34:09.949 --> 34:12.449 the kind of things that you'd experience are studying for a 34:12.447 --> 34:15.417 tough exam, you got relationship issues 34:15.422 --> 34:17.732 going on, family things happening or 34:17.731 --> 34:20.471 whatever it happens to be--How many of you under those 34:20.469 --> 34:22.379 conditions would say you eat more? 34:22.380 --> 34:25.270 How many of you would say you eat less? 34:25.268 --> 34:28.888 Okay, so an interesting mix within this kind of class. 34:28.889 --> 34:32.699 It's interesting that the preponderant response here was 34:32.699 --> 34:33.669 to eat more. 34:33.670 --> 34:35.930 There's biology that affects that. 34:35.929 --> 34:39.499 Well there was a graduate student working with me several 34:39.501 --> 34:42.691 years--a number of years ago named Elissa Epel, 34:42.690 --> 34:45.450 here's a picture of Elissa who's now on the faculty at The 34:45.452 --> 34:47.442 University of California San Francisco. 34:47.440 --> 34:50.440 She looks like she's on TV at this point because that's a 34:50.438 --> 34:51.668 picture of her on TV. 34:51.670 --> 34:55.970 Anyway, Elissa was a terrific, terrific young scholar working 34:55.972 --> 35:00.282 with us and now has become the leading expert in the world on 35:00.275 --> 35:02.135 this particular topic. 35:02.139 --> 35:05.289 She got interested in why stress makes some people eat 35:05.288 --> 35:07.188 more and some people eat less. 35:07.190 --> 35:10.250 Depression does the same thing by the way, and there were a 35:10.251 --> 35:13.051 number of interesting reasons why this might occur. 35:13.050 --> 35:15.800 What does stress actually do that would affect somebody's 35:15.802 --> 35:16.492 food intake? 35:16.489 --> 35:21.369 Well, it could stimulate hunger so people just want to eat more. 35:21.369 --> 35:24.899 It could stimulate satiety or suppress satiety, 35:24.898 --> 35:29.118 either one depending on whether you eat more or less. 35:29.119 --> 35:32.009 If could affect the sensory--how you perceive food, 35:32.010 --> 35:36.270 so if you get a certain amount of pleasure from food x, 35:36.268 --> 35:39.208 pizza let's say, or a donut, or ice cream or 35:39.210 --> 35:43.110 whatever it happens to be under non-stress conditions, 35:43.110 --> 35:46.330 the one hypothesis is that if you're stressed your physiology 35:46.329 --> 35:49.279 changes so you derive more pleasure from those foods. 35:49.280 --> 35:53.050 It could be that the foods have a whole different biological 35:53.052 --> 35:55.422 effect then what I just mentioned, 35:55.420 --> 35:59.190 and that would the ability to calm down this arousal that gets 35:59.186 --> 36:01.406 produced in people by feeling bad. 36:01.409 --> 36:04.909 When you're stressed, your body is aroused. 36:04.909 --> 36:07.609 You've all heard of the fight or flight thing, 36:07.610 --> 36:10.140 your body is aroused, that becomes an unpleasant 36:10.141 --> 36:13.371 state for most people, and one hypothesis is you eat 36:13.369 --> 36:15.899 the food and it calms that state down. 36:15.900 --> 36:18.000 It soothes you down and makes you feel better. 36:18.000 --> 36:21.720 Elissa did a variety of studies to test these different 36:21.722 --> 36:22.622 hypothesis. 36:22.619 --> 36:25.259 We're not going to talk too much about the results now 36:25.260 --> 36:28.100 because there--they would take a long time to discuss, 36:28.099 --> 36:30.689 but they're extremely interesting and she found not 36:30.688 --> 36:33.948 only does stress affect biology through some of these mechanisms 36:33.952 --> 36:37.202 that we talked about, but also it affects things like 36:37.195 --> 36:40.435 what the body does with the nutrients that come in, 36:40.440 --> 36:43.850 and when they get stored on the body as body fat where they 36:43.847 --> 36:44.727 happen to go. 36:44.730 --> 36:48.470 There is--she has research showing that when eating is done 36:48.472 --> 36:51.962 under stress conditions and there's a Cortisol response 36:51.958 --> 36:55.378 that's related to stress--you'll learn more about that 36:55.380 --> 36:58.930 later--that the body is more likely to take that fat and 36:58.929 --> 37:02.999 store it in the abdominal region and we'll come back and mention 37:02.996 --> 37:05.446 that again as we did before. 37:05.449 --> 37:09.179 A very interesting study related to this was published 37:09.181 --> 37:13.331 just a short time ago using monkeys at a well known research 37:13.333 --> 37:16.013 facility in Atlanta called Yerkes. 37:16.010 --> 37:19.790 What they wanted-- what these researchers wanted to do is look 37:19.786 --> 37:22.756 at social position within a group of monkeys, 37:22.760 --> 37:26.450 looking at the dominant monkeys in social groups versus the 37:26.449 --> 37:27.849 subordinate monkeys. 37:27.849 --> 37:30.379 If--those of you who have had any psychology will have heard 37:30.380 --> 37:31.540 about this kind of thing. 37:31.539 --> 37:37.269 This is not an infrequently used measure of stress, 37:37.268 --> 37:40.908 and how social position affects stress and therefore affects 37:40.909 --> 37:43.439 whatever outcome you're interested in, 37:43.440 --> 37:45.800 in this case, they were interested in eating. 37:45.800 --> 37:49.300 What they found here is that they compared females in this 37:49.304 --> 37:53.064 particular study and looked at the dominant versus subordinate 37:53.056 --> 37:53.606 ones. 37:53.610 --> 37:56.270 Both groups could eat all they wanted whenever they wanted 37:56.268 --> 37:58.698 because food was freely available, that's what the ad 37:58.695 --> 37:59.345 lib means. 37:59.349 --> 38:02.189 The subordinates ate less, but not a lot less, 38:02.190 --> 38:05.440 than the dominant monkeys, and the researchers 38:05.443 --> 38:09.573 hypothesized that this might be due to the stress of being 38:09.565 --> 38:13.955 subordinate in the social group; but they were more or less 38:13.956 --> 38:17.736 maintaining a normal weight and able to function fine. 38:17.739 --> 38:21.259 They then introduced high fat, high calorie foods to these 38:21.260 --> 38:24.000 animals, and allowed both groups to eat 38:24.001 --> 38:27.231 ad lib: whatever they wanted of the high fat, 38:27.230 --> 38:29.540 high calorie foods and something very interesting 38:29.536 --> 38:30.736 happened at that point. 38:30.739 --> 38:35.159 The dominants ate a bit more of this food but not an awful lot, 38:35.159 --> 38:37.729 but the subordinates ate much more, 38:37.730 --> 38:41.330 especially at night when they're not ordinarily eating. 38:41.329 --> 38:44.849 There were--the researcher--and of course gained weight as a 38:44.851 --> 38:48.311 consequence and the researchers hypothesized that maybe the 38:48.313 --> 38:52.143 calorie dense foods became a way to soothe the stress to calm the 38:52.135 --> 38:55.115 arousal and had much more reinforcing value for the 38:55.119 --> 38:58.109 subordinate animals because of the stress they were 38:58.105 --> 39:01.205 experiencing due to their social position. 39:01.210 --> 39:05.530 Very interesting study here; so here you get a different 39:05.534 --> 39:10.824 effect of introduction of a high fat diet depending on where the 39:10.818 --> 39:14.088 organism is in the social position, 39:14.090 --> 39:18.760 another interesting phenomenon that has to do with stress. 39:18.760 --> 39:22.590 Here would be a perfect example of how environment affects 39:22.586 --> 39:25.336 biology, which in turn, affects eating. 39:25.340 --> 39:30.630 Let's talk a little bit about body composition. 39:30.630 --> 39:34.340 What does the body do with calories and how does body 39:34.344 --> 39:36.564 weight get affected by these? 39:36.559 --> 39:39.349 Again, you're going to start to see both culture, 39:39.349 --> 39:42.599 which we'll talk about later, but you talk about things 39:42.601 --> 39:46.341 external to the body and things internal to the body having big 39:46.336 --> 39:47.056 impacts. 39:47.059 --> 39:50.339 Biology is a player certainly in how people regulate their 39:50.335 --> 39:51.135 body weight. 39:51.139 --> 39:55.259 The fact is, if we took all of you and put 39:55.255 --> 40:00.735 you on exactly the same diet, the precise number of calories, 40:00.737 --> 40:03.517 the precise composition of the food, 40:03.518 --> 40:06.508 x carbohydrate, x fat, those sort of things and 40:06.507 --> 40:09.427 you all exercised exactly the same amount, 40:09.429 --> 40:12.149 and we did this under very controlled circumstances there 40:12.152 --> 40:14.932 would be large variations and what your body would do with 40:14.925 --> 40:15.845 those calories. 40:15.849 --> 40:18.589 On a given calorie level, some of you might lose weight 40:18.590 --> 40:20.470 and some of you might gain weight, 40:20.469 --> 40:23.749 some of you would stay the same, and if you plotted--did a 40:23.751 --> 40:26.801 scatter plot of what would happen to your weight under 40:26.802 --> 40:29.932 identical conditions, there would be pretty large 40:29.932 --> 40:30.722 variability. 40:30.719 --> 40:33.589 That's all the biology that you're born with. 40:33.590 --> 40:38.110 Your biology determines to a great extent how your body 40:38.114 --> 40:41.634 handles calories, how efficiently it stores them, 40:41.626 --> 40:45.346 how it burns them off, how it wastes them through body 40:45.349 --> 40:47.419 heat, what it does with your basal 40:47.422 --> 40:50.312 metabolic rate that I'll describe in a moment and the 40:50.306 --> 40:50.746 like. 40:50.750 --> 40:53.610 This has been shown a number of ways. 40:53.610 --> 40:56.630 Weight is driven, as you know, 40:56.634 --> 40:58.724 by energy balance. 40:58.719 --> 41:00.559 It's calories in and calories out. 41:00.559 --> 41:04.129 Calories as we mentioned before are just a measure of energy, 41:04.130 --> 41:05.380 the unit of energy. 41:05.380 --> 41:10.210 If energy intake is different then the energy expenditure the 41:10.208 --> 41:14.878 difference is felt more or less--most often in the body fat 41:14.876 --> 41:19.136 stores and that determines whether people lose or gain 41:19.143 --> 41:21.803 weight, and as you can imagine, 41:21.802 --> 41:24.932 energy balance is affected in the following way: 41:24.934 --> 41:28.804 if intake is less then expenditure weight loss occurs, 41:28.800 --> 41:32.140 so this is of course where hunger and starvation come in. 41:32.139 --> 41:35.739 Weight gain occurs under these conditions and you have stable 41:35.739 --> 41:38.319 weight if intake and expenditure line up. 41:38.320 --> 41:43.060 Now you have problems with the top two features here in 41:43.059 --> 41:47.419 different parts of the world, overnutrition in some parts, 41:47.422 --> 41:50.672 undernutrition in others, and it's the energy balance 41:50.672 --> 41:53.272 that's helping drive the weight itself. 41:53.268 --> 41:56.548 Let's talk about metabolism for a minute. 41:56.550 --> 41:59.480 The word metabolism comes up a lot. 41:59.480 --> 42:02.940 There are metabolism diets; people say I have fast 42:02.942 --> 42:06.862 metabolism, you have slow metabolism, people use that word 42:06.860 --> 42:07.480 a lot. 42:07.480 --> 42:08.560 What does it mean? 42:08.559 --> 42:11.859 Well metabolism is essentially the term that describes how your 42:11.858 --> 42:13.028 body handles energy. 42:13.030 --> 42:17.280 When you consume food, metabolic processes occur that 42:17.278 --> 42:22.258 then--that are pertaining to the way your body is handling the 42:22.262 --> 42:25.392 incoming energy, the incoming nutrients, 42:25.387 --> 42:28.137 and all of this is our metabolic processes and 42:28.141 --> 42:30.651 metabolism is the end product of that. 42:30.650 --> 42:36.090 This would be--this particular chart here shows calories people 42:36.094 --> 42:40.844 burn, the energy expenditure part of the energy balance 42:40.838 --> 42:41.978 equation. 42:41.980 --> 42:46.050 How many calories are you burning just sitting there? 42:46.050 --> 42:49.410 If you stand up would you burn more calories? 42:49.409 --> 42:52.059 If you walked across the room would you burn more? 42:52.059 --> 42:54.549 If you ran a marathon how many would you burn? 42:54.550 --> 42:58.850 How many calories is your body burning just by virtue of being 42:58.846 --> 42:59.406 alive? 42:59.409 --> 43:02.229 These are all parts of the energy balance equation, 43:02.226 --> 43:03.856 part of energy expenditure. 43:03.860 --> 43:06.920 Let's look at the left hand side of this bar and we're going 43:06.920 --> 43:08.530 to see how it all partitions. 43:08.530 --> 43:12.560 What this particular bar shows is that this might be typical 43:12.559 --> 43:15.289 energy expenditure for an average person, 43:15.293 --> 43:17.483 so say 2,000 calories a day. 43:17.480 --> 43:20.420 Now by the way, that's why when you see the 43:20.416 --> 43:23.976 dietary recommendations, that the average person eat 43:23.981 --> 43:26.151 about 2,000 calories a day. 43:26.150 --> 43:28.770 It would be in balance: you're burning as much as you 43:28.771 --> 43:30.741 eat, you're not going to gain weight. 43:30.739 --> 43:35.199 The pieces of this, first BMR, basal metabolic 43:35.195 --> 43:35.885 rate. 43:35.889 --> 43:39.659 That's how many calories--how much energy your body is using 43:39.655 --> 43:40.865 just to function. 43:40.869 --> 43:44.349 So your cells are dividing, your heart is beating, 43:44.349 --> 43:47.169 your blood is pumping, there's some movement that 43:47.168 --> 43:50.808 occurs even if you're not--if you're being pretty sedentary. 43:50.809 --> 43:56.569 It's just the body's need to function, is the basal metabolic 43:56.568 --> 43:57.238 rate. 43:57.239 --> 44:04.419 You can see that of the 2,000 calories, that constitutes a lot 44:04.418 --> 44:05.358 of it. 44:05.360 --> 44:08.530 Now the sleeping metabolic rate, which is less then the 44:08.530 --> 44:11.230 basal metabolic rate, is how much energy you're 44:11.231 --> 44:12.701 burning during sleep. 44:12.699 --> 44:15.999 You'd be burning less during sleep because of course you're 44:16.001 --> 44:19.591 not being as active and the body doesn't need as much energy. 44:19.590 --> 44:23.450 This TEF is called the thermic effect of food. 44:23.449 --> 44:27.189 When you eat a meal or you eat any food your body takes in the 44:27.188 --> 44:31.248 calories and makes use of it, but it requires bodily 44:31.246 --> 44:35.026 processes to do that, and those bodily processes 44:35.025 --> 44:37.585 require energy for your stomach to work, 44:37.590 --> 44:39.320 your intestines to work and the like. 44:39.320 --> 44:42.180 That accounts for the little bit of the energy expenditure 44:42.177 --> 44:42.977 but not a lot. 44:42.980 --> 44:47.760 The SPA is spontaneous physical activity. 44:47.760 --> 44:52.750 You can see from this graph that not an awful lot of the 44:52.746 --> 44:57.276 calories that people burn overall are driven by how 44:57.279 --> 45:00.089 physically active they are. 45:00.090 --> 45:03.760 Now being physically active is terribly important with 45:03.764 --> 45:06.404 regulation of the balance of energy. 45:06.400 --> 45:09.660 So it's really important, but as important or even much 45:09.655 --> 45:12.655 more important, is the biology people are born 45:12.659 --> 45:16.369 with that affects things like their basal metabolic rate. 45:16.369 --> 45:19.489 That's why all of you on exactly the same diet, 45:19.489 --> 45:22.459 exactly the same exercise plan would not have the same thing 45:22.456 --> 45:25.366 happen to your weights because of the differences in things 45:25.371 --> 45:26.881 like basal metabolic rate. 45:26.880 --> 45:29.340 Some people are very efficient with their calories, 45:29.340 --> 45:32.690 as we mentioned before, they consume calories-a certain 45:32.693 --> 45:35.553 amount of calories, the body doesn't waste them 45:35.554 --> 45:38.494 through heat and by unnecessary metabolic processes; 45:38.489 --> 45:42.199 they store those calories in the body fat when it becomes 45:42.199 --> 45:43.989 more then the body needs. 45:43.989 --> 45:47.399 Other people are less efficient with calories, 45:47.402 --> 45:51.732 they're consuming calories but their body is wasting them, 45:51.726 --> 45:52.936 if you will. 45:52.940 --> 45:56.540 Now wasting the calories through high-level metabolic 45:56.541 --> 46:00.351 processes is a good thing in a culture of abundance, 46:00.349 --> 46:04.209 but it's a bad thing if starvation is a factor in one's 46:04.208 --> 46:04.708 life. 46:04.710 --> 46:07.040 Then you want to be as efficient with the calories as 46:07.036 --> 46:07.526 possible. 46:07.530 --> 46:12.410 Overall, the amount of physical activity people get as a 46:12.411 --> 46:17.301 function of overall energy expenditure is not as much as 46:17.295 --> 46:19.795 you might think, but important, 46:19.797 --> 46:22.027 very important, I don't want to underplay that, 46:22.030 --> 46:24.970 but these other factors are very important as well in 46:24.969 --> 46:26.779 determining what people weigh. 46:26.780 --> 46:30.110 If we look at energy expenditure components they 46:30.108 --> 46:34.358 change depending on whether a person is physically active. 46:34.360 --> 46:37.980 We look at the three major things: the thermic effect of 46:37.983 --> 46:41.373 food which is a small part, the energy expenditure from 46:41.369 --> 46:43.449 physical energy, and then resting energy 46:43.451 --> 46:46.381 expenditure which is essentially the basal metabolic rate. 46:46.380 --> 46:51.140 For a sedentary person that might burn 1,800 calories a day, 46:51.139 --> 46:53.399 the partitioning will look like this, 46:53.400 --> 46:57.010 but for somebody who's more physically active and might burn 46:57.007 --> 47:00.677 400 more calories a day, say by walking four miles, 47:00.679 --> 47:03.509 running four miles, or doing other things, 47:03.507 --> 47:06.067 the partitioning would look like this and so the 47:06.068 --> 47:08.838 expenditure, the orange part of the circles 47:08.836 --> 47:11.616 here from physical activity goes up a lot, 47:11.619 --> 47:15.409 by 400 calories to be precise, but it also becomes a greater 47:15.413 --> 47:18.953 percentage of the overall energy expenditure picture. 47:18.949 --> 47:21.459 That's why being physically active is such a good idea. 47:21.460 --> 47:26.740 We'll talk about body composition as well. 47:26.739 --> 47:30.809 Weight is one index of how healthy people are but also how 47:30.809 --> 47:33.949 much body fat they have is important as well, 47:33.951 --> 47:35.381 and that varies. 47:35.380 --> 47:38.680 You can have people at a given weight who vary a lot in their 47:38.675 --> 47:39.825 amount of body fat. 47:39.829 --> 47:43.439 It's the relative amount of fat and lean body tissue, 47:43.440 --> 47:46.180 lean body tissue is the combination of muscle, 47:46.179 --> 47:48.759 bones and organs, but people usually refer to 47:48.755 --> 47:52.435 lean body mass as muscle because that's the most important thing 47:52.442 --> 47:57.022 that you have some control over; and then that compares to body 47:57.016 --> 47:59.756 fat, so percent body fat of course 47:59.757 --> 48:04.247 would be a person weighs 100 pounds and if 30% of it is fat 48:04.253 --> 48:07.203 then their percent body fat is 30%. 48:07.199 --> 48:10.139 There are different ways of measuring body fat. 48:10.139 --> 48:13.939 Three of the primary ones are by looking at body fat skin 48:13.938 --> 48:17.938 folds, by weighing people under water, and by using a device 48:17.940 --> 48:19.840 called the Dexa machine. 48:19.840 --> 48:24.140 Here's what they look like, the Dexa machine on the bottom, 48:24.139 --> 48:27.209 the top left is measuring body fat with skin folds, 48:27.210 --> 48:30.040 through skin folds, and there is a device called 48:30.041 --> 48:33.661 skin fold calipers that you see a researcher there using on a 48:33.655 --> 48:37.385 part of the back there where a pinch of body fat is made, 48:37.389 --> 48:40.439 the thickness of it is measured with the calipers. 48:40.440 --> 48:42.680 That's done on several places in the body, 48:42.679 --> 48:45.529 and then you can use equations to figure out from that, 48:45.530 --> 48:48.890 estimate from that how much body fat a person actually has. 48:48.889 --> 48:52.139 The most precise way, the gold standard of measuring 48:52.141 --> 48:55.971 this, is to weigh people under water which is what you see in 48:55.967 --> 48:57.177 the upper left. 48:57.179 --> 49:00.199 A person sits on a chair, is submerged in water for just 49:00.204 --> 49:03.024 a very short time, and their body fat can get 49:03.021 --> 49:06.731 estimated from that because lean tissue and fat tissue have 49:06.730 --> 49:10.120 different densities and that can be determined through 49:10.119 --> 49:11.269 calculations. 49:11.268 --> 49:14.658 Of course you could imagine how this is difficult to do with 49:14.659 --> 49:17.879 large numbers of people and if you have to have the right 49:17.875 --> 49:18.675 equipment. 49:18.679 --> 49:20.499 The same would be true of the Dexa machine, 49:20.500 --> 49:23.810 which provides a pretty good estimate of body fat as well, 49:23.809 --> 49:27.229 but then requires lots of money and things like that. 49:27.230 --> 49:31.390 Even doing skin fold calipers, which doesn't require the 49:31.389 --> 49:34.549 expensive equipment, it requires time and training, 49:34.554 --> 49:36.934 and so if you're doing a large population study, 49:36.929 --> 49:38.469 say with thousands and thousands of people, 49:38.472 --> 49:39.392 that becomes a barrier. 49:39.389 --> 49:43.779 So you can see why most studies just measure body weight in 49:43.777 --> 49:48.157 people, even if they're just reporting it themselves rather 49:48.164 --> 49:50.514 then at measuring body fat. 49:50.510 --> 49:54.240 Then those measures get put into this thing called the body 49:54.242 --> 49:55.082 mass index. 49:55.079 --> 49:57.599 Let's talk now about what that is. 49:57.599 --> 50:00.969 There's been the need to develop an approximation of body 50:00.974 --> 50:04.454 fat because body fat, percent body fat is a stronger 50:04.445 --> 50:07.275 predictor of health then body weight is, 50:07.280 --> 50:11.090 because with body weight you get false positives and false 50:11.094 --> 50:13.844 negatives as I'll describe in a moment. 50:13.840 --> 50:16.570 You can't measure body fat all the time because it costs too 50:16.570 --> 50:17.820 much, it's too difficult. 50:17.820 --> 50:21.530 So the body mass index was developed. 50:21.530 --> 50:26.430 What it is, it's an equation that takes weight and height and 50:26.432 --> 50:31.502 puts them together into a number that is--then becomes a health 50:31.498 --> 50:36.238 standard and the number is an estimate of how much body fat 50:36.239 --> 50:37.709 people have. 50:37.710 --> 50:41.310 It's not an estimate of percent body fat but it better reflects 50:41.306 --> 50:44.956 body fat then if all you know about somebody is their weight. 50:44.960 --> 50:47.400 If you get somebody's height and their weight, 50:47.400 --> 50:49.930 you create the body mass index, which is weight, 50:49.929 --> 50:54.379 in this case expressed in kilograms divided by height 50:54.378 --> 50:58.168 squared, expressed as meters and then 50:58.170 --> 51:03.870 you come away with numbers that usually range from 12, 51:03.869 --> 51:07.299 13 at the very low end up into the 60s or even higher at the 51:07.298 --> 51:08.228 very high end. 51:08.230 --> 51:13.110 In this particular graphic you see what the classifications are 51:13.110 --> 51:15.470 for underweight, normal weight, 51:15.471 --> 51:18.621 and overweight with body mass index. 51:18.619 --> 51:21.509 Now you guys have probably heard of this concept, 51:21.510 --> 51:24.830 you've seen charts with it, you may have calculated your 51:24.827 --> 51:28.567 own body mass index or--plenty of places you can do that on the 51:28.568 --> 51:32.488 web--and you can get a sense therefore about what this means. 51:32.489 --> 51:35.219 This is a better measure then just body fat. 51:35.219 --> 51:38.869 Now body fat and body composition are usually alike. 51:38.869 --> 51:40.109 But not always. 51:40.110 --> 51:44.180 That's where you get the false positives and false negatives. 51:44.179 --> 51:46.879 For example, you can get exception like this 51:46.882 --> 51:48.832 football player, for example. 51:48.829 --> 51:52.209 If all you knew was his weight you would assume the guy weighs 51:52.213 --> 51:54.563 too much, but he's very heavily muscled 51:54.556 --> 51:57.156 so that would be a false positive for obesity, 51:57.157 --> 51:57.907 let's say. 51:57.909 --> 52:03.419 You can have cases where weight can be good but body composition 52:03.423 --> 52:05.643 is bad, so that would mean somebody is 52:05.637 --> 52:07.817 actually normal weight but they're in bad physical 52:07.824 --> 52:09.814 condition, they don't have much muscle, 52:09.811 --> 52:12.051 and they have more fat then would be optimal. 52:12.050 --> 52:14.630 Of course you can have the opposite where weight could 52:14.628 --> 52:17.448 appear to be bad but body composition is actually good, 52:17.449 --> 52:20.509 so that's why you need something that is an estimate of 52:20.514 --> 52:21.144 body fat. 52:21.139 --> 52:25.979 Here was a graph that shows the relationship between body mass 52:25.978 --> 52:30.028 index and actual body fat measured by something like 52:30.025 --> 52:31.925 underwater weighing. 52:31.929 --> 52:34.429 You get a large sample of people, in this particular 52:34.429 --> 52:36.989 study, each dot represents a subject 52:36.989 --> 52:40.279 in here and you take their body mass index, 52:40.280 --> 52:43.430 just by knowing their height and weight and you see how 52:43.434 --> 52:45.484 carefully it represents body fat. 52:45.480 --> 52:49.160 The women are in the lighter colored, the orange colored 52:49.164 --> 52:52.654 squares and the men are in the blue colored ovals. 52:52.650 --> 52:57.490 The lines here show that--several things. 52:57.489 --> 53:00.489 This is the average for the women, the line that goes right 53:00.490 --> 53:02.770 through there; this would be the average for 53:02.768 --> 53:04.418 the man that goes through there. 53:04.420 --> 53:07.370 What you see is a pretty nice relationship that, 53:07.369 --> 53:12.259 as body mass index increases body fat increases as well, 53:12.260 --> 53:14.970 and if this were a perfect predictor, 53:14.969 --> 53:17.799 if body mass index were a perfect estimator of body fat 53:17.798 --> 53:20.888 you'd see an exactly straight line that would go at an angle 53:20.889 --> 53:23.299 right like this, but it would be straight. 53:23.300 --> 53:26.940 So these lines are almost straight but there is some curve 53:26.940 --> 53:27.580 in them. 53:27.579 --> 53:30.189 So it's not perfect but it's pretty good. 53:30.190 --> 53:33.150 You can see here that women have more body fat then men, 53:33.148 --> 53:35.838 that's a natural thing, and I'll describe that in a 53:35.838 --> 53:38.468 minute; and also there is some 53:38.465 --> 53:41.145 variability in these things. 53:41.150 --> 53:44.030 Let's just take a couple of data points as an example. 53:44.030 --> 53:45.670 Let's see. 53:45.670 --> 53:49.530 Let's look at the squares for women that you see in the 53:49.525 --> 53:50.235 circles. 53:50.239 --> 53:53.819 Here would be a case of individual subjects in this 53:53.820 --> 53:57.540 study who have the same body mass index but have much 53:57.543 --> 53:59.193 different body fat. 53:59.190 --> 54:02.670 This shows that there's some error in this, 54:02.670 --> 54:05.030 that's it's not a perfect measure of body fat, 54:05.030 --> 54:09.380 because two women who have body mass index of about 20 which is 54:09.382 --> 54:13.532 in the healthy range have as little as about 12% body fat or 54:13.525 --> 54:14.855 as much as 40%. 54:14.860 --> 54:17.390 This would be examples of somebody who's weights are about 54:17.394 --> 54:19.984 the same at a given height but they're in different kind of 54:19.976 --> 54:22.286 physical condition, so there's some error there. 54:22.289 --> 54:25.879 There's also error in this direction where you'd have two 54:25.876 --> 54:28.626 people with the same amount of body fat, 54:28.630 --> 54:31.160 but would have a different body mass index, 54:31.159 --> 54:33.329 and they're quite different from one another. 54:33.329 --> 54:36.579 There are different errors in this but for the most part body 54:36.579 --> 54:39.289 mass index is about the best predictor we have, 54:39.289 --> 54:43.219 short of doing physiological assessment of what body fat 54:43.224 --> 54:44.804 really is in people. 54:44.800 --> 54:47.980 A few composition--facts about body composition, 54:47.978 --> 54:51.088 as I said, women have--naturally have more body 54:51.090 --> 54:52.240 fat then men. 54:52.239 --> 54:55.829 That is probably due to the need to preserve energy for 54:55.833 --> 54:59.233 pregnancy and lactation, I mentioned that before. 54:59.230 --> 55:02.700 Strength training increases muscle mass in the body and that 55:02.699 --> 55:06.169 changes body composition even if the amount of fat stays the 55:06.170 --> 55:06.700 same. 55:06.699 --> 55:09.979 Muscle is more metabolically active then fat. 55:09.980 --> 55:13.170 It takes more energy for the body to deal with a pound of 55:13.172 --> 55:15.912 muscle then it does with a pound of body fat, 55:15.909 --> 55:18.859 so that's why things like strength training become a very 55:18.864 --> 55:22.034 good idea if one's concerned about body weight regulation. 55:22.030 --> 55:25.010 It's good for lots of different reasons but that would be one of 55:25.009 --> 55:25.339 them. 55:25.340 --> 55:30.910 Normal body fat is about 15% to 17% of body weight for men, 55:30.911 --> 55:36.581 20% to 25% body fat for women would be the typical range. 55:36.579 --> 55:39.169 As I mentioned before, men and women tend to store 55:39.170 --> 55:42.550 weight in different parts of the body, and you have the apple and 55:42.554 --> 55:43.564 the pear shape. 55:43.559 --> 55:47.399 I also mentioned before, there are exceptions to this 55:47.402 --> 55:47.922 rule. 55:47.920 --> 55:50.880 When people gain enough weight they tend to gain weight all 55:50.880 --> 55:52.980 over the body, but when people begin gaining 55:52.978 --> 55:55.478 weight it's more likely for women to distribute it below the 55:55.478 --> 55:56.748 waist, men above the waist. 55:56.750 --> 56:01.220 There's good and bad news for both men and women in this. 56:01.219 --> 56:05.389 The good news for the men, is that the weight stored above 56:05.385 --> 56:09.545 the waist seems more readily mobilized when people go on a 56:09.552 --> 56:10.212 diet. 56:10.210 --> 56:13.650 The bad news is that it's worse for your health to have the 56:13.653 --> 56:16.093 weight stored in that part of the body. 56:16.090 --> 56:18.190 For the women, the weight stored below the 56:18.190 --> 56:20.960 waist seems little--it's more difficult to mobilize. 56:20.960 --> 56:24.960 It's protected more heavily by the body, it's defended if you 56:24.961 --> 56:27.421 will; but the good news is that it's 56:27.422 --> 56:31.212 not going to create risk for disease as much as the excess 56:31.208 --> 56:33.398 weight stored above the waist. 56:33.400 --> 56:37.160 As I mentioned before, the reason that people think 56:37.164 --> 56:40.744 this is the case, is that the fat below the waist 56:40.735 --> 56:44.795 is a storage depot for energy in preparation for pregnancy, 56:44.800 --> 56:47.490 for all the energy needed to sustain a pregnancy, 56:47.489 --> 56:50.049 and then to feed the child, breastfeed the child after 56:50.050 --> 56:50.390 that. 56:50.389 --> 56:53.469 The female is burning tremendous number of calories 56:53.474 --> 56:56.564 doing these sort of things as you might imagine; 56:56.559 --> 57:00.259 and that is the part of the body where the fat for that 57:00.257 --> 57:02.447 purpose has the energy stored. 57:02.449 --> 57:08.029 Because the reproduction is so much--is such an important 57:08.027 --> 57:12.157 function to defend, the body defends that way in 57:12.159 --> 57:16.599 particular compared to the apple shape that might occur above the 57:16.596 --> 57:17.216 waist. 57:17.219 --> 57:20.229 So a very interesting part of biology of that. 57:20.230 --> 57:23.090 Let's talk about genes and body weight. 57:23.090 --> 57:26.990 How much is body weight affected by genes? 57:26.989 --> 57:31.649 Well it's affected a lot by behavior of course-how much one 57:31.646 --> 57:32.206 eats. 57:32.210 --> 57:35.770 It's affected a lot by culture, I mean we've got rampant things 57:35.766 --> 57:37.886 like obesity in the United States, 57:37.889 --> 57:40.569 you've got almost none of it in a country like Somalia, 57:40.570 --> 57:44.610 but then biology is also an important player potentially. 57:44.610 --> 57:48.830 I'd like to have you guys do a little vote with your clickers 57:48.829 --> 57:52.409 and I'd like you to estimate--now somebody responded 57:52.414 --> 57:56.214 already before I've described the question but maybe it 57:56.213 --> 57:57.693 doesn't matter. 57:57.690 --> 58:02.340 How much of the variation in body weight in a population is 58:02.336 --> 58:04.256 explained by genetics? 58:04.260 --> 58:09.130 Now that doesn't mean--so 0 to 25% doesn't mean that 25% of 58:09.126 --> 58:13.986 people who are overweight are overweight because of genetic 58:13.992 --> 58:15.002 reasons. 58:15.000 --> 58:19.570 What it means is that in a population that either weighs a 58:19.574 --> 58:22.744 lot or a little, how much is driven by genetics, 58:22.742 --> 58:25.972 and as weight varies within a population how much is driven by 58:25.974 --> 58:26.614 genetics. 58:26.610 --> 58:30.640 Do you think its zero to 25%, which would mean genetics are 58:30.639 --> 58:33.209 having a pretty small contribution; 58:33.210 --> 58:36.200 25% to 40%; 40% to 70%; 58:36.199 --> 58:39.109 or 70% to 100%, that would mean genetics is 58:39.112 --> 58:42.652 having a very strong contribution to body weight? 58:42.650 --> 58:47.130 Okay. 58:47.130 --> 58:48.260 Let's see. 58:48.260 --> 58:49.600 Everybody done voting? 58:49.599 --> 58:51.479 Let's see what your graph says. 58:51.480 --> 58:57.030 14% of you said zero to 25%. 58:57.030 --> 59:01.370 The majority or more then anything else said 25% to 40%, 59:01.369 --> 59:04.709 40% to 70%, 39% and then a few said 70% to 100%, 59:04.710 --> 59:07.130 so most of you are clustered in the middle somewhere. 59:07.130 --> 59:08.940 Well let's see what the number really is. 59:08.940 --> 59:12.760 There are several prominent researchers who have done 59:12.762 --> 59:15.632 studies on genetics and body weight, 59:15.630 --> 59:18.890 and you'll hear some of them referred to later in the class, 59:18.889 --> 59:22.299 and I'm going to show a video clip today that has one of the 59:22.297 --> 59:23.567 figures interviewed. 59:23.570 --> 59:26.990 This is Rudy Leibel who is a very well known physician 59:26.994 --> 59:30.684 researcher at Columbia University who has studied this. 59:30.679 --> 59:34.119 This is Albert Stunkard at The University of Pennsylvania who 59:34.121 --> 59:37.051 did the initial human genetic studies on body weight 59:37.047 --> 59:40.137 regulation and Claude Bouchard is at a place called The 59:40.144 --> 59:43.364 Pennington Institute and he's one of the world leaders on 59:43.356 --> 59:44.616 these as well. 59:44.619 --> 59:47.559 II'm going to show you some data from studies that these 59:47.559 --> 59:49.109 folks and others have done. 59:49.110 --> 59:52.330 There are different methods for studying the genetic 59:52.326 --> 59:54.026 contribution to anything. 59:54.030 --> 59:59.530 If you're interested in how much genes contribute to hair 59:59.534 --> 1:00:01.514 color, to depression, 1:00:01.512 --> 1:00:04.402 to schizophrenia, to heart disease, 1:00:04.400 --> 1:00:07.710 to cancer, or to body weight regulation these are the kind of 1:00:07.706 --> 1:00:09.576 methods that typically get used. 1:00:09.579 --> 1:00:12.199 First, you can do animal studies where you can actually 1:00:12.195 --> 1:00:14.705 go in and measure genes, you can do that in humans of 1:00:14.713 --> 1:00:15.443 course too. 1:00:15.440 --> 1:00:19.630 Or you can manipulate genes by breeding animals to be a certain 1:00:19.634 --> 1:00:20.044 way. 1:00:20.039 --> 1:00:22.299 You'll see some examples of that. 1:00:22.300 --> 1:00:26.660 You can do family studies to see how well--how much certain 1:00:26.659 --> 1:00:28.839 things cluster in families. 1:00:28.840 --> 1:00:32.190 Let's just say you look to see whether--if parents are 1:00:32.193 --> 1:00:35.553 overweight whether they're more or less likely to have 1:00:35.547 --> 1:00:38.467 overweight children, and so you can see the 1:00:38.472 --> 1:00:39.942 clustering in families. 1:00:39.940 --> 1:00:43.500 Now that's easy to do from a research point of view but has 1:00:43.498 --> 1:00:45.808 weaknesses as well, as you can imagine because the 1:00:45.811 --> 1:00:47.611 families also share culture and environment, not just the 1:00:47.606 --> 1:00:47.956 biology. 1:00:47.960 --> 1:00:55.360 Adoption studies are an interesting issue here as well. 1:00:55.360 --> 1:01:00.960 That's when you look at people adopted versus their biological 1:01:00.956 --> 1:01:03.156 relatives who are not. 1:01:03.159 --> 1:01:06.109 Identical and fraternal twins, twins reared together and 1:01:06.106 --> 1:01:09.426 apart, and then of course you can go in and do gene studies. 1:01:09.429 --> 1:01:11.249 Now I'd like to describe a couple of these. 1:01:11.250 --> 1:01:14.980 First, there are ways you can breed obesity and in this case 1:01:14.983 --> 1:01:18.653 there is a laboratory strain of mice called the OB/OB mouse 1:01:18.653 --> 1:01:21.793 that's bred to be obese, so you can look at the genes 1:01:21.791 --> 1:01:22.771 that are doing this. 1:01:22.768 --> 1:01:24.908 Here are more examples of that kind of thing. 1:01:24.909 --> 1:01:27.589 These have been used for years in laboratory studies. 1:01:27.590 --> 1:01:30.390 I'd like to show you this little video clip, 1:01:30.389 --> 1:01:32.609 in this case, Rudolph Leibel, 1:01:32.610 --> 1:01:36.180 the researcher I mentioned is interviewed, 1:01:36.179 --> 1:01:39.439 but it talks a little bit about this issue, about biology. 1:01:39.440 --> 1:01:40.340 > 1:01:40.340 --> 1:02:43.480 1:02:43.480 --> 1:02:45.230 Interesting here that he talks about cultural perceptions, 1:02:45.230 --> 1:02:47.920 that we accept the fact that height is very genetically 1:02:47.922 --> 1:02:50.822 determined but less so with weight because of the way we've 1:02:50.815 --> 1:02:52.955 been trained to think about this issue. 1:02:52.960 --> 1:02:55.630 But in fact, biology is an important player. 1:02:55.630 --> 1:02:58.860 One of the first studies done with humans on this was done by 1:02:58.858 --> 1:03:01.608 Stunkard, one of the researchers I mentioned before, 1:03:01.605 --> 1:03:03.215 in the country of Denmark. 1:03:03.219 --> 1:03:07.249 Denmark keeps very careful records of adoptees and for 1:03:07.250 --> 1:03:11.970 research reasons keeps track of people who are adopted later in 1:03:11.965 --> 1:03:15.915 their life and also the parents who raise them, 1:03:15.920 --> 1:03:17.880 and the parents who gave birth to them, 1:03:17.880 --> 1:03:19.610 the biological versus adopted parents. 1:03:19.610 --> 1:03:22.570 Stunkard, collaborating with Danish researchers, 1:03:22.565 --> 1:03:25.705 was able to look at these adoptees, people who were 1:03:25.708 --> 1:03:28.098 adopted at birth; and then as adults, 1:03:28.097 --> 1:03:31.357 how much they weigh and looked to see whether their weights 1:03:31.360 --> 1:03:34.040 were closer, more closely resembled those of 1:03:34.041 --> 1:03:37.321 their biological parents or the parents who raised them. 1:03:37.320 --> 1:03:40.360 If the relationship was strong between the adoptees' adult 1:03:40.358 --> 1:03:42.648 weights and the parents who raised them, 1:03:42.650 --> 1:03:45.230 it would be a strong argument for culture and environment. 1:03:45.230 --> 1:03:48.020 If the relationship is stronger with the biological parents, 1:03:48.021 --> 1:03:50.011 of course then it would argue for genes. 1:03:50.010 --> 1:03:54.350 The results look like this. 1:03:54.349 --> 1:03:57.909 If we look at the right first, the relationship between 1:03:57.913 --> 1:04:01.813 weights of the children and their mothers and fathers--their 1:04:01.806 --> 1:04:05.896 adoptive mothers and fathers--as the weights increase here from 1:04:05.898 --> 1:04:09.528 thin to the median level, to overweight and obese you 1:04:09.527 --> 1:04:12.277 don't see a very clear relationship because as the 1:04:12.284 --> 1:04:15.704 adoptees are getting heavier, the parents aren't necessarily 1:04:15.701 --> 1:04:16.171 heavier. 1:04:16.170 --> 1:04:19.110 So this would suggest that there's not a very strong 1:04:19.110 --> 1:04:22.740 relationship between the parents who raised the children and the 1:04:22.744 --> 1:04:25.704 weights of the adoptees; whereas you get a different 1:04:25.697 --> 1:04:27.477 picture for the biological parents. 1:04:27.480 --> 1:04:30.440 As the weights are going up in the adoptees, 1:04:30.440 --> 1:04:33.570 the weights of the biological parents are higher too, 1:04:33.570 --> 1:04:36.040 even though there had never been any contact after birth. 1:04:36.039 --> 1:04:39.569 So this would be a strong argument for biology. 1:04:39.570 --> 1:04:43.870 Another very interesting way of looking at this is comparing 1:04:43.867 --> 1:04:47.797 monozygotic and dizygotic twins: identical twins versus 1:04:47.800 --> 1:04:50.820 fraternal twins, so of course twins born at the 1:04:50.820 --> 1:04:53.560 same time to the same parents, raised in the same 1:04:53.559 --> 1:04:56.179 environment--but the monozygotic twins, 1:04:56.179 --> 1:04:58.199 the identical twins, share all their genetic 1:04:58.202 --> 1:04:58.722 material. 1:04:58.719 --> 1:05:02.139 The dizygotic twins share half of their genetic material. 1:05:02.139 --> 1:05:05.729 You can look at how the weights of twin pairs relate to each 1:05:05.731 --> 1:05:09.021 other, how the twins relate to each other in weight. 1:05:09.018 --> 1:05:12.408 If biology is a player you'd expect a stronger relationship 1:05:12.411 --> 1:05:15.161 for the monozygotic twins then the dizygotic. 1:05:15.159 --> 1:05:17.759 In fact that's what you find. 1:05:17.760 --> 1:05:22.230 This is a picture showing twin pairs of dizygotic twins, 1:05:22.230 --> 1:05:24.990 so twins born same time to the same parents, 1:05:24.989 --> 1:05:27.349 but share half their genetic materials, 1:05:27.349 --> 1:05:29.579 and each panel here shows one pair of twins, 1:05:29.579 --> 1:05:31.269 so this would be one pair, another pair, 1:05:31.268 --> 1:05:31.528 etc. 1:05:31.530 --> 1:05:35.360 You can really see pretty striking differences in--some in 1:05:35.356 --> 1:05:38.376 height but also in the amount of body fat, 1:05:38.380 --> 1:05:41.660 how-- and also other physical characteristics, 1:05:41.659 --> 1:05:43.969 so you see big differences here, you see big differences 1:05:43.965 --> 1:05:44.465 here, etc. 1:05:44.469 --> 1:05:48.649 Then if you look at a picture that shows monozygotic twins who 1:05:48.648 --> 1:05:52.828 share all their genetic material then what you tend to get are 1:05:52.827 --> 1:05:55.087 mirror images of one another. 1:05:55.090 --> 1:05:58.220 It's another method of estimating the genetic 1:05:58.217 --> 1:06:00.347 contributions to this issue. 1:06:00.349 --> 1:06:02.839 So genes are a player to be sure. 1:06:02.840 --> 1:06:06.790 I'm going to skip over this and conclude about some of the 1:06:06.788 --> 1:06:07.548 genetics. 1:06:07.550 --> 1:06:12.240 Those of you who voted for 25% to 40% were correct. 1:06:12.239 --> 1:06:15.559 That is the amount of variance in the population that you can 1:06:15.561 --> 1:06:16.781 explain by genetics. 1:06:16.780 --> 1:06:19.410 Now the question is, is that a lot or a little? 1:06:19.409 --> 1:06:21.799 Depends on your perspective. 1:06:21.800 --> 1:06:26.050 If the culture believes that body weight is completely under 1:06:26.048 --> 1:06:30.388 personal control, or driven by environmental 1:06:30.385 --> 1:06:34.015 factors, then hearing this number sounds 1:06:34.016 --> 1:06:37.126 like a lot; that the biology is really a 1:06:37.134 --> 1:06:41.044 player and we weren't expecting it to be that way. 1:06:41.039 --> 1:06:45.629 On the other hand, this means that less then half 1:06:45.632 --> 1:06:51.472 of the explanation of population body weight is biological and 1:06:51.471 --> 1:06:52.621 genetic. 1:06:52.619 --> 1:06:56.629 There's at least 60%, and as much as 75% left over to 1:06:56.630 --> 1:07:00.410 explain--be explained by cultural type factors. 1:07:00.409 --> 1:07:04.829 It depends on your perspective, but here are some of the 1:07:04.833 --> 1:07:09.663 conclusions that people have raised putting genetics and body 1:07:09.661 --> 1:07:11.271 weight together. 1:07:11.268 --> 1:07:15.258 Genetic influences largely determine whether a person can 1:07:15.260 --> 1:07:18.290 become obese, but it is the environment that 1:07:18.288 --> 1:07:22.248 determines whether a person does become obese and that extent of 1:07:22.253 --> 1:07:23.263 that obesity. 1:07:23.260 --> 1:07:27.140 Then another similar quote, Genes load the gun and the 1:07:27.143 --> 1:07:29.493 environment pulls the trigger. 1:07:29.489 --> 1:07:33.109 So you may have very little obesity in a country like 1:07:33.114 --> 1:07:37.164 Somalia where starvation and famine become real issues, 1:07:37.159 --> 1:07:40.179 but if their environment became like ours, 1:07:40.179 --> 1:07:43.289 then genetics might determine how much weight an individual 1:07:43.286 --> 1:07:46.336 would gain but the environment would determine whether the 1:07:46.340 --> 1:07:48.430 whole population is gaining weight. 1:07:48.429 --> 1:07:52.059 We'll come back and talk about culture and environment later. 1:07:52.059 --> 1:07:55.139 I'd like to end with a discussion of something on taste 1:07:55.143 --> 1:07:58.803 aversions because this also is a very interesting phenomenon that 1:07:58.800 --> 1:08:01.600 has to do with the way our body handles food. 1:08:01.599 --> 1:08:05.509 How many of you have a strong aversion to at least some food? 1:08:05.510 --> 1:08:09.580 Okay, most of you said yes. 1:08:09.579 --> 1:08:12.929 Now my guess is that if we had you write down what those foods 1:08:12.927 --> 1:08:16.167 are it would highly different from one another because these 1:08:16.166 --> 1:08:19.126 things tend to get conditioned in by happenstance. 1:08:19.130 --> 1:08:21.440 You tend to eat a certain food when you're sick, 1:08:21.435 --> 1:08:24.275 the body then doesn't want you to eat that food anymore. 1:08:24.279 --> 1:08:27.019 There's a good evolutionary reason for that of course, 1:08:27.020 --> 1:08:29.630 because in nature if you eat something that makes you sick it 1:08:29.626 --> 1:08:31.186 means that it's not good for you, 1:08:31.189 --> 1:08:34.249 and there's potentially toxins and poison in that, 1:08:34.250 --> 1:08:37.310 and so you want to avoid that food as much as possible. 1:08:37.310 --> 1:08:42.190 A very interesting example of this was done with coyotes and 1:08:42.188 --> 1:08:42.848 sheep. 1:08:42.850 --> 1:08:45.430 In the Western part of the U.S. 1:08:45.430 --> 1:08:49.180 the sheep farmers are plagued by coyotes eating the sheep and 1:08:49.180 --> 1:08:52.180 this is--this becomes a real problem for them. 1:08:52.180 --> 1:08:54.610 So the question is how do you control this process? 1:08:54.609 --> 1:08:58.839 The farmers and--the sheep farmers and the animal 1:08:58.837 --> 1:09:03.327 protection people don't like each other so much, 1:09:03.328 --> 1:09:05.708 because the farmers want to kill the coyotes and the animal 1:09:05.711 --> 1:09:07.601 protection people don't want this to happen. 1:09:07.600 --> 1:09:11.380 A psychological researcher came up with a very interesting way 1:09:11.376 --> 1:09:13.846 to deal with this, having to do with food 1:09:13.853 --> 1:09:14.723 aversions. 1:09:14.720 --> 1:09:18.970 What they did was they took sheep--like carcasses of dead 1:09:18.970 --> 1:09:22.540 sheep that the coyotes would ordinarily eat, 1:09:22.538 --> 1:09:25.488 and laced the carcass with lithium chloride--which when 1:09:25.494 --> 1:09:28.944 eaten by the coyotes would make them very sick--It wouldn't kill 1:09:28.943 --> 1:09:31.953 them but it would make them throw up and be nauseous and 1:09:31.953 --> 1:09:34.203 feel terrible, and all kinds of stuff like 1:09:34.198 --> 1:09:34.908 that. 1:09:34.908 --> 1:09:39.138 What they found then is the coyotes wouldn't eat the sheep 1:09:39.137 --> 1:09:39.877 anymore. 1:09:39.880 --> 1:09:43.330 The same kind of food aversion that you guys may have 1:09:43.332 --> 1:09:47.192 experienced got drilled into the coyotes by this particular 1:09:47.185 --> 1:09:48.775 aversive experience. 1:09:48.779 --> 1:09:52.769 There are video tapes--I tried to get one but I couldn't find 1:09:52.770 --> 1:09:56.430 one--of coyotes coming up to baby lambs and running away 1:09:56.430 --> 1:10:00.290 whimpering as if that little lamb was going to do something 1:10:00.288 --> 1:10:03.548 terrible to them because of this aversion, 1:10:03.550 --> 1:10:06.100 so you can see how strong the biology becomes. 1:10:06.100 --> 1:10:09.540 Okay, so we will see you guys on Monday. 1:10:09.538 --> 1:10:12.778 Don't forget to turn in your concept sheets outside the room. 1:10:12.779 --> 1:10:17.999